"

Sensation and Perception

Introduction

Imagine standing on a city street corner. You might be struck by movement everywhere as cars and people go about their business, by the sound of a street musician’s melody or a horn honking in the distance, by the smell of exhaust fumes or of food being sold by a nearby vendor, and by the sensation of hard pavement under your feet.

We rely on our sensory systems to provide important information about our surroundings. We use this information to successfully navigate and interact with our environment so that we can find nourishment, seek shelter, maintain social relationships, and avoid potentially dangerous situations. But although sensory information is critical to our survival, there is so much information available at any given time that we would be overwhelmed if we were forced to attend to all of it. In fact, we are aware of only a fraction of the sensory information taken in by our sensory systems at any given time.

Here, we will discuss the difference between sensation and perception, before an overview of how sensory information is received and processed by the nervous system and how that affects our conscious experience of the world.

It is important to remember that sensation and perception is built upon the premise that we have a system in place that can respond to physical stimuli in our environment. Indeed, let’s emphasize the physical aspect of the stimuli. These stimuli have characteristics that include many words that you likely encountered in a physics course – energy, waves, pressure, etc. Therefore, our sensory systems must have cells that can respond to such components in order to send the signal, via neural communication, to our brains in order to result in analysis for perception.

“Psychology: Chapter 5: Sensation and Perception” by OpenStax. https://learn.saylor.org/pluginfile.php/40708/mod_resource/content/1/OpenStax-Psychology.pdf. Licensed under CC-BY.

Sensation and Perception

Fundamentals of Sensing and Perceiving

Sensation

What does it mean to sense something? Sensory receptors are specialized neurons that respond to specific types of stimuli. When sensory information is detected by a sensory receptor, sensation has occurred.

For example, light that enters the eye causes chemical changes in cells that line the back of the eye. These cells relay messages, in the form of action potentials (as you learned when studying biopsychology), to the central nervous system. The conversion from sensory stimulus energy to action potential is known as transduction.

You have probably known since elementary school that we have five senses: vision, hearing (audition), smell (olfaction), taste (gustation), and touch (somatosensation). It turns out that this notion of five senses is oversimplified. We also have sensory systems that provide information about balance (the vestibular sense), body position and movement (proprioception and kinesthesia), pain (nociception), and temperature (thermoception).

The sensitivity of a given sensory system to the relevant stimuli can be expressed as an absolute threshold. Absolute threshold refers to the minimum amount of stimulus energy that must be present for the stimulus to be detected 50% of the time. Another way to think about this concept is by asking how dim can a light be or how soft can a sound be and still be detected half of the time. The sensitivity of our sensory receptors can be quite amazing. It has been estimated that on a clear night, the most sensitive sensory cells in the back of the eye can detect a candle flame 30 miles away (Okawa & Sampath, 2007). Under quiet conditions, the hair cells (the receptor cells of the inner ear) can detect the tick of a clock 20 feet away (Galanter, 1962).

It is also possible for us to get messages that are presented below the threshold for conscious awareness—these are called subliminal messages. A stimulus reaches a physiological threshold when it is strong enough to excite sensory receptors and send nerve impulses to the brain: This is an absolute threshold. A message below that threshold is said to be subliminal: We receive it, but we are not consciously aware of it. Over the years there has been a great deal of speculation about the use of subliminal messages in advertising, rock music, and self-help audio programs.

Research evidence shows that in laboratory settings, people can process and respond to information outside of awareness. But this does not mean that we obey these messages like zombies; in fact, hidden messages have little effect on behavior outside the laboratory (Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc, 1980; Rensink, 2004; Nelson, 2008; Radel, Sarrazin, Legrain, & Gobancé, 2009; Loersch, Durso, & Petty, 2013).

Absolute thresholds are generally measured under incredibly controlled conditions in situations that are optimal for sensitivity. Sometimes, we are more interested in how much difference in stimuli is required to detect a difference between them. This phenomenon is known as the just noticeable difference (jnd) or difference threshold. Unlike the absolute threshold, the difference threshold changes depending on the stimulus intensity.

As an example, imagine yourself in a very dark movie theater. If an audience member were to receive a text message on her cell phone which caused her screen to light up, chances are that many people would notice the change in illumination in the theater. However, if the same thing happened in a brightly lit arena during a basketball game, very few people would notice. The cell phone brightness does not change, but its ability to be detected as a change in illumination varies dramatically between the two contexts. Ernst Weber proposed this theory of change in difference threshold in the 1830s, and it has become known as Weber’s law: The difference threshold is a constant fraction of the original stimulus, as the example illustrates.

“Psychology: Chapter 5: Sensation and Perception” by OpenStax. https://learn.saylor.org/pluginfile.php/40708/mod_resource/content/1/OpenStax-Psychology.pdf. Licensed under CC BY.

Perception

Although our sensory receptors are constantly collecting information from the environment, it is ultimately how we interpret that information that affects how we interact with the world. Perception refers to the way sensory information is organized, interpreted, and consciously experienced. Perception involves both bottom-up and top-down processing.

Bottom-up processing refers to the fact that perceptions are built from sensory input. On the other hand, how we interpret those sensations is influenced by our available knowledge, our experiences, and our thoughts. This ability is called top-down processing. One way to think of this concept is that sensation is a physical process, whereas perception is psychological.

For example, upon walking into a kitchen and smelling the scent of baking cinnamon rolls, the sensation is the scent receptors detecting the odor of cinnamon, but the perception may be “Mmm, this smells like the bread Grandma used to bake when the family gathered for holidays.” Although our perceptions are built from sensations, not all sensations result in perception. In fact, we often don’t perceive stimuli that remain relatively constant over prolonged periods of time. This process is known as sensory adaptation.

Imagine entering a classroom with an old analog clock. Upon first entering the room, you can hear the ticking of the clock; as you begin to engage in conversation with classmates or listen to your professor greet the class, you are no longer aware of the ticking. The clock is still ticking, and that information is still affecting sensory receptors of the auditory system. The fact that you no longer perceive the sound demonstrates sensory adaptation and shows that while closely associated, sensation and perception are different.

There is another factor that affects sensation and perception: attention. Attention plays a significant role in determining what is sensed versus what is perceived. Imagine you are at a party full of music, chatter, and laughter. You get involved in an interesting conversation with a friend, and you tune out all the background noise. If someone interrupted you to ask what song had just finished playing, you would probably be unable to answer that question.

One of the most interesting demonstrations of how important attention is in determining our perception of the environment occurred in a famous study conducted by Daniel Simons and Christopher Chabris (1999). In this study, participants watched a video of people dressed in black and white passing basketballs. Participants were asked to count the number of times the team in white passed the ball. During the video, a person dressed in a black gorilla costume walks among the two teams. You would think that someone would notice the gorilla, right? Nearly half of the people who watched the video didn’t notice the gorilla at all, despite the fact that he was clearly visible for nine seconds. Because participants were so focused on the number of times the white team was passing the ball, they completely tuned out other visual information.

Failure to notice something that is completely visible because of a lack of attention is called inattentional blindness. In a similar experiment, researchers tested inattentional blindness by asking participants to observe images moving across a computer screen. They were instructed to focus on either white or black objects, disregarding the other color. When a red cross passed across the screen, about one third of subjects did not notice it (Figure 5.2) (Most, Simons, Scholl, & Chabris, 2000)

Nearly one third of participants in a study did not notice that a red cross passed on the screen because their attention was focused on the black or white figures. This image shows a male looking at a board that has different shapes in black and white, and one cross that is red. (credit: Cory Zanker)

 

“Psychology: Chapter 5: Sensation and Perception” by OpenStax. Retrieved from https://learn.saylor.org/pluginfile.php/40708/mod_resource/content/1/OpenStax-Psychology.pdf. Licensed under CC-BY.

Motivation can also affect perception. Have you ever been expecting a really important phone call and, while taking a shower, you think you hear the phone ringing, only to discover that it is not? If so, then you have experienced how motivation to detect a meaningful stimulus can shift our ability to discriminate between a true sensory stimulus and background noise. The ability to identify a stimulus when it is embedded in a distracting background is called signal detection theory. This concept might also explain why a mother is awakened by a quiet murmur from her baby, but not by other sounds that occur while she is asleep.

Signal detection theory has practical applications, such as increasing air traffic controller accuracy. Controllers need to be able to detect planes among many signals (blips) that appear on the radar screen and follow those planes as they move through the sky. In fact, the original work of the researcher who developed signal detection theory was focused on improving the sensitivity of air traffic controllers to plane blips (Swets, 1964). Our perceptions can also be affected by our beliefs, values, prejudices, expectations, and life experiences. Importantly, individuals who are deprived of the experience of binocular vision during critical periods of development have trouble perceiving depth (Fawcett, Wang, & Birch, 2005).

The shared experiences of people within a given cultural context can have pronounced effects on perception. For example, Marshall Segall, Donald Campbell, and Melville Herskovits (1963) published the results of a multinational study in which they demonstrated that individuals from Western cultures were more prone to experience certain types of visual illusions than individuals from non-Western cultures, and vice versa. One such illusion that Westerners were more likely to experience was the Müller-Lyer illusion: The lines appear to be different lengths, but they are actually the same length.

In the Müller-Lyer illusion, lines appear to be different lengths although they are identical. (a) Arrows at the ends of lines may make the line on the right appear longer, although the lines are the same length. (b) When applied to a three-dimensional image, the line on the right again may appear longer although both black lines are the same length.

“Psychology: Chapter 5: Sensation and Perception” by OpenStax. Retrieved fromhttps://learn.saylor.org/pluginfile.php/40708/mod_resource/content/1/OpenStax-Psychology.pdf. Licensed under CC-BY.

These perceptual differences were consistent with differences in the types of environmental features experienced on a regular basis by people in a given cultural context. People in Western cultures, for example, have a perceptual context of buildings with straight lines, what Segall’s study called a carpentered world (Segall et al., 1966). In contrast, people from certain non-Western cultures with an uncarpentered view, such as the Zulu of South Africa, whose villages are made up of round huts arranged in circles, are less susceptible to this illusion (Segall et al., 1999).

It is not just vision that is affected by cultural factors. Research has demonstrated that the ability to identify an odor, and rate its pleasantness and its intensity, varies cross-culturally (Ayabe-Kanamura, Saito, Distel, Martínez-Gómez, & Hudson, 1998). Children described as thrill seekers are more likely to show taste preferences for intense sour flavors (Liem, Westerbeek, Wolterink, Kok, & de Graaf, 2004), which suggests that basic aspects of personality might affect perception. Furthermore, individuals who hold positive attitudes toward reduced-fat foods are more likely to rate foods labelled as reduced fat as tasting better than people who have less positive attitudes about these products (Aaron, Mela, & Evans, 1994).

“Psychology: Chapter 5: Sensation and Perception” by OpenStax. Retrieved from https://learn.saylor.org/pluginfile.php/40708/mod_resource/content/1/OpenStax-Psychology.pdf. Licensed under CC-BY.

Overview of Vision and Audition

Visual and auditory stimuli both occur in the form of waves. Although the two stimuli are very different in terms of composition, wave forms share similar characteristics that are especially important to our visual and auditory perceptions. Here, we will describe the physical properties of the waves as well as the perceptual experiences associated with them.

Amplitude and Wavelength

Two physical characteristics of a wave are amplitude and wavelength. The amplitude of a wave is the height of a wave as measured from the highest point on the wave (peak or crest) to the lowest point on the wave (trough). Wavelength refers to the length of a wave from one peak to the next.

This image shows a horizontal wave length. The top of the wave is labelled peak, the bottom of the wave is labelled trough, and the space in-between waves is labelled wavelength. The horizontal line cutting through the entire wave is labelled amplitude.

Assessment Questions: “Psychology: Chapter 5: Sensation and Perception” by OpenStax. Retrieved fromhttps://learn.saylor.org/pluginfile.php/40708/mod_resource/content/1/OpenStax-Psychology.pdf. Licensed under CC-BY.

Wavelength is directly related to the frequency of a given wave form. Frequency refers to the number of waves that pass a given point in a given time period and is often expressed in terms of hertz (Hz), or cycles per second. Longer wavelengths will have lower frequencies, and shorter wavelengths will have higher frequencies.

This figure illustrates waves of differing wavelengths/frequencies. At the top of the figure, the red wave has a long wavelength/short frequency. Moving from top to bottom, the wavelengths decrease and frequencies increase.

“Psychology: Chapter 5: Sensation and Perception” by OpenStax. Retrieved fromhttps://learn.saylor.org/pluginfile.php/40708/mod_resource/content/1/OpenStax-Psychology.pdf. Licensed under CC-BY.

“Psychology: Chapter 5: Sensation and Perception” by OpenStax. Retrieved from https://learn.saylor.org/pluginfile.php/40708/mod_resource/content/1/OpenStax-Psychology.pdf. Licensed under CC-BY.

 

Light Waves

The visible spectrum is the portion of the larger electromagnetic spectrum that we can see. The electromagnetic spectrum encompasses all of the electromagnetic radiation that occurs in our environment and includes gamma rays, x-rays, ultraviolet light, visible light, infrared light, microwaves, and radio waves. The visible spectrum in humans is associated with wavelengths that range from 380 to 740 nm—a very small distance, since a nanometer (nm) is one billionth of a meter. Other species can detect other portions of the electromagnetic spectrum.

For instance, honeybees can see light in the ultraviolet range (Wakakuwa, Stavenga, & Arikawa, 2007), and some snakes can detect infrared radiation in addition to more traditional visual light cues (Chen, Deng, Brauth, Ding, & Tang, 2012; Hartline, Kass, & Loop, 1978)

In humans, light wavelength is associated with perception of color. Within the visible spectrum, our experience of red is associated with longer wavelengths, greens are intermediate, and blues and violets are shorter in wavelength. An easy way to remember this is the mnemonic ROYGBIV: red, orange, yellow, green, blue, indigo, violet. The amplitude of light waves is associated with our experience of brightness or intensity of color, with larger amplitudes appearing brighter.

Different wavelengths of light are associated with our perception of different colors. This image shows a horizontal bar that starts off darker on each end and moves to brighter colors towards the middle. The bar is labelled as wavelength (nm) from left to right: 400, 500, 600 and 700. The following labels are included below the bar from left to right: cosmic radiation, gamma rays, x-rays, ultraviolet, infrared, terhertz radiation, radar, television and radio broadcasting, and AC circuits. (credit: modification of work by Johannes Ahlmann)

“Psychology: Chapter 5: Sensation and Perception” by OpenStax. Retrieved fromhttps://learn.saylor.org/pluginfile.php/40708/mod_resource/content/1/OpenStax-Psychology.pdf. Licensed under CC-BY.

“Psychology: Chapter 5: Sensation and Perception” by OpenStax. Retrieved from https://learn.saylor.org/pluginfile.php/40708/mod_resource/content/1/OpenStax-Psychology.pdf. Licensed under CC-BY.

 

Sound Waves

Like light waves, the physical properties of sound waves are associated with various aspects of our perception of sound. The frequency of a sound wave is associated with our perception of that sound’s pitch. High-frequency sound waves are perceived as high-pitched sounds, while low-frequency sound waves are perceived as low-pitched sounds. The audible range of sound frequencies is between 20 and 20000 Hz, with greatest sensitivity to those frequencies that fall in the middle of this range.

As was the case with the visible spectrum, other species show differences in their audible ranges. For instance, chickens have a very limited audible range, from 125 to 2000 Hz. Mice have an audible range from 1000 to 91000 Hz, and the beluga whale’s audible range is from 1000 to 123000 Hz. Our pet dogs and cats have audible ranges of about 70–45000 Hz and 45–64000 Hz, respectively (Strain, 2003).

The loudness of a given sound is closely associated with the amplitude of the sound wave. Higher amplitudes are associated with louder sounds. Loudness is measured in terms of decibels (dB), a logarithmic unit of sound intensity. A typical conversation would correlate with 60 dB; a rock concert might check in at 120 dB. A whisper 5 feet away or rustling leaves are at the low end of our hearing range; sounds like a window air conditioner, a normal conversation, and even heavy traffic or a vacuum cleaner are within a tolerable range.

However, there is the potential for hearing damage from about 80 dB to 130 dB: These are sounds of a food processor, power lawnmower, heavy truck (25 feet away), subway train (20 feet away), live rock music, and a jackhammer. The threshold for pain is about 130 dB, a jet plane taking off or a revolver firing at close range (Dunkle, 1982).

Although wave amplitude is generally associated with loudness, there is some interaction between frequency and amplitude in our perception of loudness within the audible range. For example, a 10 Hz sound wave is inaudible no matter the amplitude of the wave. A 1000 Hz sound wave, on the other hand, would vary dramatically in terms of perceived loudness as the amplitude of the wave increased.

Of course, different musical instruments can play the same musical note at the same level of loudness, yet they still sound quite different. This is known as the timbre of a sound. Timbre refers to a sound’s purity, and it is affected by the complex interplay of frequency, amplitude, and timing of sound waves.

“Psychology: Chapter 5: Sensation and Perception” by OpenStax. Retrieved from https://learn.saylor.org/pluginfile.php/40708/mod_resource/content/1/OpenStax-Psychology.pdf. Licensed under CC-BY.

Sensation and Perception – Video

CrashCourse. (2014, March 3). Sensation & perception – Crash course psychology #5. [Video File]. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=unWnZvXJH2o. Standard YouTube License.

Bozeman Science. (2012, March 17). The sensory system. [Video File]. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=TAzTFgPSPiU. Standard YouTube License.

Summary

Sensation occurs when sensory receptors detect sensory stimuli. Perception involves the organization, interpretation, and conscious experience of those sensations. All sensory systems have both absolute and difference thresholds, which refer to the minimum amount of stimulus energy or the minimum amount of difference in stimulus energy required to be detected about 50% of the time, respectively.

Sensory adaptation, selective attention, and signal detection theory can help explain what is perceived and what is not. In addition, our perceptions are affected by a number of factors, including beliefs, values, prejudices, culture, and life experiences.

Both light and sound can be described in terms of wave forms with physical characteristics like amplitude, wavelength, and timbre. Wavelength and frequency are inversely related so that longer waves have lower frequencies, and shorter waves have higher frequencies.

In the visual system, a light wave’s wavelength is generally associated with color, and its amplitude is associated with brightness. In the auditory system, a sound’s frequency is associated with pitch, and its amplitude is associated with loudness.

“Psychology: Chapter 5: Sensation and Perception” by OpenStax. Retrieved from https://learn.saylor.org/pluginfile.php/40708/mod_resource/content/1/OpenStax-Psychology.pdf. Licensed under CC-BY.Assessment Questions:

“Psychology: Chapter 5: Sensation and Perception” by OpenStax. Retrieved from https://learn.saylor.org/pluginfile.php/40708/mod_resource/content/1/OpenStax-Psychology.pdf. Licensed under CC-BY.

License

PSY101 Introduction to Psychology Copyright © by Open Bay Path University. All Rights Reserved.